The Science and Psychology of Infant-Toddler Care: How an Understanding of Early Learning Has Transformed Child Care |
WWW.VIRTUALLABSCHOOL.ORG
ACTIVITY ID: 20671
Name
Certifier
Date
|
---|---|
Source: The following summary is wholly based on articles by Lally, J. R. (2009, November). The science and psychology of infant-toddler care. ZERO TO THREE; and Lally, J. R. (2012, Jan-Mar). Want success in school? Start with babies! Kappa Delta Pi Record; and Lally, J. R., Mangione, P. (2017, May). Caring relationships: the heart of early brain development. Young Children. It takes time for the science of infant development to filter down to child care programs. We now understand that babies come into care with their own agenda for learning, their own hard-wired thirst for knowledge, and using all of their senses. Their discoveries are not siloed into separate domains like language, emotional, and intellectual, but instead infants experiences inform multiple parts of their development. Babies brains are designed to learn from their caregivers, and so their apparent helplessness functions as a way to strengthen bonds with their caregivers. Its important to balance what we know about how babies learn with infant and toddler caregiving practices in child care programs. Lally notes: As shown by research compiled in From Neurons to Neighborhoods (National Research Council & Institute of Medicine, 2000) and other sources (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994; Honig, 2002; Sroufe, 1996), babies seek out their caregivers for help with their physical survival; emotional security; the provision of a safe base for learning, modeling, mentoring, and regulation of social behavior; information about the workings of the world; and appropriate rules for living. They also exhibit their own skills as inventors, communication initiators, imitators, interpreters, integrators, meaning seekers, relationship builders, and curious, motivated, self-starting learners. And, because of the way the young child is wired for this learning, unique adult accommodations are required. (2009) Alison Gopnik, a leading researcher on infant cognition, showed that babies learn differently than school age children, and must be treated differently than older children. Other research indicates that babies do not thrive in environments designed specifically for older children, and actually become less interested in learning, expect less of themselves, and are more stressed and less cooperative. (Lally citing Gopnik 2009a). Though older children can learn through adult-directed learning targets, babies learn best through opportunities to explore anything new, unexpected, or informative. This explains why replications of school-age environments and experiences do not work for babies. In light of this understanding, infant teachers now pay attention and adjust their interactions with babies to accommodate babies interests into their learning experiences. For example, a teacher might notice a babys fascination with looking at her own hands and fingers and engage with the baby using elements of story, her fingers, or a prop such as a bright red ball and talk back and forth in an exchange using warm tones and clear, simple words, thereby attending to the childs physical, social, emotional, and intellectual needs. This type of interaction takes into account babies active role in learning and treats them as active participants in learning rather than passive audiences. Child care programs that understand how babies develop also appreciate that relationships and experiences with trusted caregivers are the base for all learning. This secure relationship is understood to be foundational for a childs positive trajectory, including helping a child feel secure about learning, their self image, and relationships with others. Such centers include hazard-free learning areas that are appealing and cozy, and caregivers who understand infant development, and offer opportunities for children to explore without interruption. Program Best Practices to 勛圖厙 Infant Learning and Development
Forming AttachmentsAs well as forming attachments to parents, infants can and do form attachments with caregivers. Lack of secure attachments in care can have negative effects, such as increased stress and decreased learning. (Dalli, 1999; Honig, 1998; Raikes, 1993, 1996). Belsky, Spritz, & Crnic (1994) and Honig, (2002) believe that the security a child feels in their early attachments is related to their positive self image and later relationships.
Sense of SelfHow teachers behave, what they do, how they talk about their own feelings, how they conduct themselves around other teachers, all these interactions influence the infants teachers care for. These small interactions affect how children behave and the way children learn to relate to others. (Bornstein, & Bornstein, 1995).
Including FamiliesFamilies are childrens first teacher and the way families influence a childs care are integrated into child care practices. Child care practices that fail to do this risk causing a child to feel torn or confused between their family practices and program practices. At worst, the child rejects their familys practices. Integrating a familys care practices in child care helps children feel supported in their family identity and secure in their care inside and outside their family.
Different Practices Along the Birth 36 Month TimelineDuring the first 36 months, children are interested in security, exploration, and self. These topics are more prominent at different times throughout a childs first three years. See Greenspans Stages of Emotional Development below. Greenspans Stages of Emotional Development Summarized
Source: Lally, R.J. (2009) cites: From S. I. Greenspan, Emotional Development in Infants and Toddlers in Infant/Toddler Caregiving: A Guide to Social-Emotional Growth and Socialization (pp. 1518), by J. R. Lally (Ed.), 1990, Sacramento: California Department of Education. Originally published in First Feelings: Milestones in the Emotional Development of Your Baby and Child, by S. I. Greenspan, 1985, New York: Viking. Copyright by S. I. Greenspan. Condensed with permission. Discussed more fully in Infancy and Early Childhood: The Practice of Clinical Assessment and Intervention with Emotional and Developmental Challenges, by S. I. Greenspan, 1992, Madison, CT: International University Press.
Responsive PracticesIndividualized care meets each unique childs needs and interests, and encourages further curiosity, exploration, and growth. Responsive practices are accommodating, receptive, thoughtful, and flexible. When using responsive practices, teachers:
Emergent CurriculumAn emergent curriculum accepts that most all children come into the world with their own learning goals and that following a childs lead keeps them on a continuum of learning throughout their lives. Teachers have an opportunity to encourage infants to continue to be self-directed learners and explorers. Researchers Raikes & Pope Edwards (2009) emphasize how important it is for infants development to let them direct their own learning choices and agenda. Teachers can support this by:
The Curriculum Planning Process, which is used in an ongoing fashion, looks like this: Observe -> Document -> Assess -> Reflect & Plan -> Implement -> repeat The teacher uses this framework to collect data, test hypotheses, and flexibly implement any needed adaptations or updates. This process is flexible enough that it can be used to implement any adaptations or corrections in any planning timeframe: whether in the moment, daily, or weekly. It does not have to follow this cyclical pattern illustrated above, but can be interactive among all steps. Action Still NeededWhat we know to be best practices for optimal infant development are not yet implemented at the majority of child care centers in the United States. The reasons for this are complex and include the high cost of providing quality care and high staff turnover rates. Bolstered by information about brain development, including the extraordinary brain development of children age two and younger (Lally, 2012), we can champion what we know to be best practices for this important group of people as more people begin to appreciate the long-term benefits of quality early child care. These practices will help infants and toddlers build strong relationships. Neuroscience informs us that early experiences affect brain architecture (Spence, Shapiro, and Zaidel, 1996), and the benefits of such practices in infant care settings will extend into childrens life trajectory and may counteract adverse experiences. Author information about J. Ronald Lally:RONALD LALLY, EdD, is co-director of the Center for Child and Family Studies, WestEd, and the Program for Infant Toddler Care (PITC), as well as one of the founders of ZERO TO THREE. His research deals with social-emotional development in infancy and the impact of early intervention on adult functioning. He is the executive producer more than 20 Program for Infant/Toddler Care (PITC) infanttoddler training DVDs. These DVDs and accompanying print materials are the most widely distributed infanttoddler care training materials in the country. He and his staff have been responsible for the development of infanttoddler and preschool guidelines and standards for the states of California and Ohio. Related Resources cited in original article by J. Ronald Lally:The Program for Infant/Toddler Care The Web site of The Program for Infant Toddler Care offers information, resources, and training opportunities for infant/toddler caregivers and links to PITC publications distributed by the California Department of Education. Key publications containing information about quality infanttoddler care are: Concepts for Care: 20 Essays on Infant/Toddler Development and Learning Edited by J. Ronald Lally, Peter L. Mangione & Deborah Greenwald (Editors). San Francisco: WestEd. California Infant Toddler Learning & Development Foundations & DVDs (2009). Sacramento, CA: CDE Press. California Infant Toddler Learning & Development Program Guidelines & DVDs (2009). Sacramento, CA: CDE Press. Sources cited in original article by J. Ronald Lally: ReferencesBelsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment: Theory and evidence. In M. Rutter & D. Hay (Eds.), Development through life (pp. 373402). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Belsky, J., Spritz, B., & Crnic, K. (1994). Infant attachment security and affective-cognitive information processing at age 3. Psychological Science, 7(2), 111114. Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Heslington, M., Gini, M., Suwalsky, J. T. D., Venuti, P., et al. (2008). Mother-child emotional availability in ecological perspective: Three countries, two regions, two genders. Developmental Psychology, 44, 666680. Bornstein, M., & Bornstein, H. (1995). Caregivers responsiveness and cognitive development in infants and toddlers: Theory and research. In L. Mangione (Ed.), Infant/Toddler Caregiving: A Guide to Cognitive Development and Learning (pp. 1221). Sacramento: California Department of Educations Bureau of Publications. Buffett Early Childhood Fund & Ounce of Prevention Fund. (2009). Educare. Retrieved September 16, 2009, from www.buffettearlychildhoodfund.org/educare.html Dalli, C. (1999, September). Starting childcare: What young children learn about relating to adults in the first weeks of settling into a childcare centre. Paper presented at the Early Childhood Convention, Nelson, New Zealand. Early Head Start. (2009). Early Head Start National Resource Center. Retrieved September 16, 2009, from www.ehsnrc.org/ Edwards, C. P., & Raikes, H. (2002). Extending the dance: Relationship-based approaches to infant/toddler care and education. Young Children, 57(4), 1017. Gopnik, A. (2009a). The philosophical baby: What childrens minds tell us about truth, love, and the meaning of life. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux. Gopnik, A. (2009b, August 16). Your baby is smarter than you think. The New York Times, p. WK10. Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: Minds, brains, and how children learn. Fairfield, NJ: William Morrow. Greenspan, S. I. (1990). Emotional development in infants and toddlers. In J. R. Lally (Ed.), nfant/toddler caregiving: A guide to social-emotional growth and socialization (pp. 1518). Sacramento: California Department of Education. Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., & Krauss, M. W. (2001). Children with disabilities. A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(3, Serial No. 266). Honig, A. S. (1998, August). Attachment and relationships: Beyond parenting. Paper presented at the Head Start Quality Network Research Satellite Conference, East Lansing, Michigan. Honig, A. S. (2002). Secure relationships: Nurturing infant/toddler attachment in early care settings. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Lally, J. R. (2003). Infant-toddler child care in the United States: Where has it been? Where is it now? Where is it going? Zero to Three, 24(1),&紳莉莽梯;2934. Lally, J. R., & Mangione, P. L. (2006). New perspectives in infant/toddler care [DVD]. Sacramento: California Department of Education Press. Mangione, P. L. (1995). Program for infant/toddler caregivers. Infant/toddler caregiving: A guide to culturally sensitive care. Sacramento: California Department of Education and WestEd. National Research Council & Institute of Medicine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. J. P. Shonkoff & D. A. Phillips, (Eds), Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Oser, C., & Cohen, J. (2003). Americas babies: The ZERO TO THREE Policy Center data book. Washington, DC: ZERO TO THREE. Program for Infant/Toddler Care. (2009). PITCs six program policies. Retrieved September 21, 2009, from http://www.pitc.org/pub/pitc_docs/138?x-r=disp Raikes, H. (1993). Relationship duration in infant care: Time with a high-ability teacher and infantteacher attachment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8(3), 309325. Raikes, H. (1996). A secure base for babies: Applying attachment concepts to the infant care settings. Young Children, 51(5), 5967. Raikes, H., & Pope Edwards,C. (2009). Extending the dance in infant and toddler caregiving. altimore: Brookes. Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. The summary above is wholly based on this source: Lally, J. R. (2009, November). The Science and Psychology of Infant-Toddler Care. ZERO TO THREE; and Lally, J. R. (2012, Jan-Mar). Want success in school? Start with babies! Kappa Delta Pi Record; and Lally, J. R., Mangione, P. (2017, May). Caring relationships: the heart of early brain development. Young Children. |